Pop quiz on the harmful effects of junk food: If you take a classroom of hungry kids and mainline Ding Dongs into their veins, then give them a math test, what鈥檚 going to happen? How about first offering them a Skittles free-for-all? A Pop-Tart pig-out? Or a french fry-feeding frenzy?
Ask any parent to offer anecdotes about how food affects kids鈥 brains, and the floodgates open. To wit: At my daughter鈥檚 recent birthday party, 6-year-old girls decorated masks with the focus of Renaissance masters until we served the chocolate cake. Within minutes the scene had regressed 1,700 odd years. The bacchanalia of half-naked, sugar-smeared revelers, jumped on the furniture, intent on raising Hades from the underworld with their screams.
Parents in attendance marveled at the 鈥渃lassic sugar rush.鈥
But what was really happening in those little brains? Was junk food really so harmful? And what does science tell us about food鈥檚 relationship to cognition?
A famine of facts
As a firm believer in the power of food to influence kids鈥 thinking, I was eager to dive into the sea of literature on this topic.
Prepared for some serious Internet surfing on the phrase “junk food harmful,” I soon found myself wading in a veritable puddle of research. Compared say, to television鈥檚 impact on the brain, less seems to be known about how food affects children鈥檚 mental functioning.
鈥淚 was surprised too,鈥 says Kerri Tobin, an Associate Professor at Louisiana State University and researcher on the potential link between fast food and academic performance. 鈥淭here鈥檝e been plenty of studies on the effects of malnutrition, mineral deficiencies, and the importance of breakfast but not that much on foods.鈥
Sugar, fat, and additives 鈥 oh my!
Some of the findings, she says, challenge conventional wisdom. For instance, researchers have found that . Another found that children, like adults, performed better on mental cognition and memory tests when amped up on caffeine. 鈥淭hough I don鈥檛 think anyone鈥檚 suggesting that we give caffeine to children,鈥 Tobin hastens to add.
There have been fewer studies focusing on how certain foods affect cognition, though there is a growing body of research on how .聽In the past 30 years, there also have been several studies attempting to understand the effects of additives in our foods. According to one , many reports have documented that colorings, preservatives, and other additives carry adverse behavioral effects.
In 2004 a U.K. study found that . After putting a group of 3-year-olds on an additive-free diet for a week, researchers gave some of the children an additive-laced drink. Others drank an additive-free but otherwise identical beverage. Parents of the children drinking the additives reported an increase in disruptive behavior. (They didn鈥檛 know which drink their children had been given). Another published in 2007 in the British medical journal found significant adverse behavioral responses in both 3-year-olds and 8- and 9-year-olds when they consumed drinks with a mixture of sodium benzoate and artificial coloring. Yet another published in 2007 in the found that a junk food diet at age 4 陆 was associated with higher levels of hyperactivity 鈥 but the researchers noted that it was unclear whether this reflected a nutritional issue or a difference in parenting styles.
Burgers with a side of bad test scores?
Tobin鈥檚 own research 鈥 鈥 looked at the food consumption surveys of 5,500 fifth-graders from a national longitudinal survey by the Department of Education. Tobin found that children who ate fast food three or more times per week performed lower on standardized tests in reading and math.
How much lower? 鈥淥n a 1-to-100 scale, it would mean about 17 points,鈥 says Tobin. In other words, the difference between a 90 and a 73.
Think such findings say more about kids鈥 socioeconomic background than the actual effects of fast food? Not necessarily. Tobin says she controlled for many variables. These included the parents鈥 income, age, education, occupational status, and number of hours employed; whether or not they attend parent-teacher conferences; whether the family has food security issues; and how many hours the child spends in before- or aftercare. She also controlled for the children鈥檚 race, gender, and age; the number of books they own; their interest in the subject matter tested; other food behaviors; obesity; 黄色app attendance; and their geographical region and urbanicity.
According to Tobin, none of these factors changed the link between a fast-food diet and low academic performance.
So does this mean fast food makes you stupid?
Is junk food really harmful? The devil’s in the details
Tobin is cautious about jumping to any conclusions. 鈥淗aving controlled for so many things, I don鈥檛 think it鈥檚 unreasonable to imagine that there could potentially be a biological effect.鈥 But, she adds, there could be other explanations. 鈥淢aybe kids who don鈥檛 perform well academically are the type of kids who like to pretend they get to go to fast-food restaurants all the time. Or maybe the kind of parent that goes to fast-food restaurants is the same kind of parent who doesn鈥檛 have time to help with homework.鈥
In any case, Tobin鈥檚 research underscores some of the difficulties in researching the relationship between food and academic performance. Self-reported food consumption surveys (from both children and adults) are notoriously inaccurate. Moreover, most food isn鈥檛 a single ingredient you can test in isolation.
To make matters worse, testing the effects of certain ingredients requires that families adhere to special diets. This typically limits the duration of the tests and therefore their scope. Finally, because we don鈥檛 eat ingredients in isolation, understanding how chemical compounds interact in our bodies is usually beyond the scope of nutritional research. As Tobin noted, if there is something about fast food that has a deleterious effect on kids鈥 thinking, it won鈥檛 be easy to isolate. 鈥淚s it saturated fat? Is it a certain preservative? Is it the ammonia they use to clean hamburger meat? We don鈥檛 know.鈥
Still, since Tobin’s study more than ten years ago, there has been new evidence (including a ) concluding that junk food has a negative effect on children’s learning. So, when your children, having consumed targeted advertising designed to tempt their tastebuds, begs for the Big Mac and chocolate shake over what you’re cooking, you can cite their academics as one reason for saying no.